Detecting Preference and Motivation for Environmental Resources: An Animal Welfare Approach

Caroline Marques Maia

Published Date: 2016-02-17
DOI10.21767/2572-5459.100013

Caroline Marques Maia*

Laboratory of Animal Physiology and Behavior, IBB, UNESP, Botucatu, SP, Brazil, IBB, UNESP, Botucatu, SP, Brazil

Corresponding Author:
Caroline Marques Maia
Laboratory of Animal Physiology
IBB, UNESP, Botucatu, SP, Brazil
E-mail: carolmm_luzi@hotmail.com

Received date: November 26, 2015 Accepted date: February 12, 2016 Published date: February 17, 2016

Citation: Maia CM. Detecting Preference and Motivation for Environmental Resources: An Animal Welfare Approach. J Anim Res Nutr 2016, 1:13. doi: 10.21767/2572-5459.100013

Copyright: 2016 Maia CM. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Visit for more related articles at Journal of Animal Research and Nutrition

Abstract

Animal Welfare Science began to gradually develop in the 60´s, probably as a consequence of the publication of the book “Animal Machines” by Ruth Harrison [1], which denounced several practices that were done without regard for animal suffering. Since then, much research has been conducted aiming to identify the welfare states of animals. Firstly, an animal was considered to have good welfare when it was physically healthy, feeding, and producing well. Everyone can easily understand the importance of these characteristics and believe they may be part of animal welfare considerations, but welfare may not be restricted to this. How the animal behaves may also indicate if it is in a better or a worse condition. Thus, a healthy and productive animal or even an animal that is feeding well may be in a poor welfare condition if it is, for example, expressing some stereotypic behaviour. Stereotypies are defined as a series of movements regularly repeated with no apparent purpose or goal [2], probably induced by frustration, repeated attempts to cope and/or brain dysfunction.

https://betkolikgirisi.com https://betlikeguncel.com https://betparkagiris.com https://bettickett.com https://betturkeyegiris.com https://extrabetgirisi.com https://holiganbeti.com https://ilbete.com https://ikimisligirisi.com https://imajbetegir.com https://jojobeti.com https://kralbetting.com https://mariogiris.com https://marsbahise.com https://meritegiris.com https://milanobeti.com https://piabetegir.com https://redwinegiris.com https://supertotobete.com https://tempobetegir.com

Animal Welfare Science began to gradually develop in the 60 ´s, probably as a consequence of the publication of the book “Animal Machines” by Ruth Harrison [1], which denounced several practices that were done without regard for animal suffering. Since then, much research has been conducted aiming to identify the welfare states of animals. Firstly, an animal was considered to have good welfare when it was physically healthy, feeding, and producing well. Everyone can easily understand the importance of these characteristics and believe they may be part of animal welfare considerations, but welfare may not be restricted to this. How the animal behaves may also indicate if it is in a better or a worse condition. Thus, a healthy and productive animal or even an animal that is feeding well may be in a poor welfare condition if it is, for example, expressing some stereotypic behaviour. Stereotypies are defined as a series of movements regularly repeated with no apparent purpose or goal [2], probably induced by frustration, repeated attempts to cope and/or brain dysfunction [3].

However, we should consider that even an animal behaving “naturally” does not necessarily have good welfare, and that the opposite may also be true [4]. For example, an animal fleeing from a predator is behaving naturally, but it should not be considered to have good welfare in that moment. Moreover, how can we be sure that some behaviours are natural and others are unnatural, stereotypic or otherwise abnormal? How can we be sure that an animal expressing some abnormal behaviour is in a worse situation than an animal not expressing it? If we consider that the expression of an abnormal behaviour could be a coping strategy used to deal with the restrictions of the captivity [5], and that an animal not expressing it could be in such an intense state of frustration or depression that it is simply unable to do so, we will not likely be sure of our answers. Similarly, according to Hill & Broom [6], natural behaviours should not necessarily be expected in environments that are more restricted than natural ones as captivity. Moreover, some studies contested stereotypies as unequivocal indicators of poor welfare conditions (e.g. De Passillé et al. [7]).

Furthermore, some aspects of animal welfare are difficult to measure: emotional states, including positive internal states such as pleasure and negative internal states such as fear or pain. There are many papers demonstrating that different animal species are able to feel pain, including fish species (for a review about pain in fish, see Braithwaite & Huntingford, [8]), and other papers indicating that animals are often afraid of handling and human beings [9,10]. Such negative emotional states are detrimental to animal welfare, but, as I mentioned above, the emotional component of animal welfare is not restricted to unpleasant feelings. The absence of pain and any another kind of suffering is not an indicator per se that the animal is in a good welfare state. The animal may not be suffering in any way, but it might not be experiencing any pleasure either. Does this mean that the animal is in a good welfare condition? Moreover, what do we know about the pleasure animals feel, for example, when interacting with a resource they are highly motivated to reach? Much more research is necessary to identify positive emotions in animals.

Thus, identifying animal welfare indicators that are unequivocal has not been an easy task. Considering these issues, Marian Dawkins [11,12] has proposed that we should turn our attention to what the animal wants instead of looking for such welfare indicators. In this context, Volpato et al. [13] has defined good welfare as the internal state of an animal when it is in a situation that it freely chose. Dawkins [12] has extended this idea, by defining good animal welfare as a condition in which the animal is healthy and has what it wants. Thus, many papers have focused on evaluating the animal’s preferences through choice tests, where two (binary tests) or more (multiple-choice tests) options are available for the animal to choose. Considering this approach, much progress has been made. The researchers have identified the preferences of many animal species for different environmental resources, such as sucrose [14], habitats [15], sexual partners [16-19], temperature [20] and many other resources.

In addition to this idea, Duncan [21] recommended identifying not only preferred and non-preferred conditions, but also the intensity of the responses. Thus, many papers have evaluated the intensity of the animals’ motivation to access environmental resources, and some progress has also been made towards this goal. The studies have evaluated the animal motivation for different resources, such as contact with conspecifics [22]; additional space [23]; dust-baths [24]; motivation to reach many different items [25] and even to express a stereotypic behaviour [26] (e.g. Duncan and Kite, 1987 [27]) or the number of times that a pre-conditioned behaviour (such as push a switch; e.g. sherwin, [23]) needs to be repeated to access a specific resource. In these tests, fodd is usually used as a benchmark to compare the motivation of the animals to reach other resources, since it is considered the resource the animal is most motivated to access. 

Although these approaches seem to be promising, some caution is necessary to apply and interpret preference and motivation tests. For instance, an animal may choose an available option because of a bias of the test apparatus or of the surrounding environment, or even because it was distracted by something (for review, see Volpato et al. [13]). In addition, the animals may choose what is good at the moment, but not necessarily what will be good for welfare in the long-term. Considering the motivation tests, Hovland et al. [28] has demonstrated that the food resource, usually used as a benchmark, may even elicit an aversion response if it is made available in excess. Thus, using food as a parameter to compare the animal motivation to reach other resources is an approach that also needs caution. Furthermore, basically most preference and motivation studies are concentrated on farm and lab animals. Considering that zoo and other captive animals may also suffer from poor welfare conditions, it is also relevant to evaluate their preferences and motivation for the resources. When applying environmental enrichment, which is a technique widely used to improve the welfare conditions of zoo animals, why not give the animals what they prefer or are very motivated to have? Thus, although preference and motivation tests are promising in improving the welfare conditions of the animals, some caution is needed and we still have a long way to go.

Acknowledgments

I thank Dr. Rebecca Meagher for her great help with the English edition of the manuscript.

References

open access journals, open access scientific research publisher, open access publisher
Select your language of interest to view the total content in your interested language

Viewing options

Flyer image
journal indexing image

Share This Article

paper.io

agar io

wowcappadocia.com
cappadocia-hotels.com
caruscappadocia.com
brothersballoon.com
balloon-rides.net

wormax io